We have been collaborating with Dr. LaMotte of Yale University School
of Medicine in conducting psychophysical and neurophysiological studies
on the tactile perception of the microtexture, shape and softness of objects.
We have shown that humans can detect extremly fine textures composed of
50 nanometers high parallel bars etched on plane glass plates (LaMotte
and Srinivasan, 1991). Our neurophysiological recordings indicate that
when such fine textures are stroked on the fingerpad skin, the fingerprint
ridges vibrate and cause Pacinian Corpuscles to respond, thus enabling
the detection of the microtexure (Srinivasan, Whitehouse and LaMotte,
1990).
In studies of the tactile perception of shape, a series of two- and three-dimensional
objects (e.g., cylinders, spheres, ellipsoids and wavy surfaces) were
pressed or stroked at various orientations across the fingerpads of anesthetized
monkeys and evoked responses in cutaneous mechanoreceptive primary afferent
nerve fibers were recorded (Srinivasan and LaMotte, 1991; LaMotte and
Srinivasan, 1993; LaMotte, Srinivasan, Lu, and Klusch-Petersen, 1994;
LaMotte, Lu, and Srinivasan, 1995; 1996). Major geometrical properties
of the shapes were well represented in the spatio-temporal responses of
SA and RA afferent fiber populations, particularly those of the SAs. The
results show that the following hypothesis explains qualitatively all
the data we have obtained until now: the depth of indentation and the
change in curvature of the skin surface are encoded by the discharge rates
of SAs; in addition, the velocity and the rate of change in skin surface
curvature are encoded by the discharge rates of both SAs and RAs.
The intensive parameters of shapes, such as the magnitude of change in
skin curvature produced by contact with the object surface were encoded
in the discharge rates of SAs and RAs, but this neural code was also influenced
by changes in stroke velocity. Spatial parameters of the response, such
as the outline of the region of mechanoreceptor activity in the skin,
encoded the size, shape and orientation of the 2-D outline of the object
in contact with the skin. The third dimension of shape was represented
best in the shape of the distribution of spatial discharge rates primarily
in the SA fiber population. The shapes of the spatial discharge rates
of RAs were more irregular and variable than those of SAs and exhibited
poor or no representations of object shape. It was hypothesized that the
distribution of slopes (in impulses/sec/mm) of the spatial discharge rate
profile evoked by an object in the SA population encoded the distribution
of curvatures on the surface of the object in contact with the skin. This
is a neural code that is probably invariant with moderate changes in the
parameters that govern contact conditions between the object and the skin,
such as the contact force or orientation and velocity of its trajectory.
Therefore, among the different possible geometric representations of the
shape of objects, the intrinsic description, i.e., the surface curvature
as a function of the distance along the surface, seems to be relevant
for tactile sensing of shape.
Based on a theoretical analysis of the mechanics of contact, we have
proposed a mechanism by which shapes of objects within contact regions
are perceived through the tactile sense. The curvature of the skin surface
under an object, which we know from differential geometry is approximated
by the second spatial derivative of surface deflection, is coded without
differentiating (which is a noise enhancing process), but by exploiting
its relationship to surface pressure. Pressure peaks occur where the depths
of indentation and/or changes in the skin surface curvature are high.
The skin effectively acts as a low-pass filter in transmitting the mechanical
signals, and the mechanoreceptors respond to the blurred versions of the
surface pressure distribution, thus encoding the shape of the object in
terms of its surface curvatures (Srinivasan and LaMotte, 1991).
We have also shown that the human discriminability of softness or compliance
of objects depends on whether the object has a deformable or rigid surface
(Srinivasan and LaMotte, 1995; 1996). When the surface is deformable,
the spatial pressure distribution within the contact region is dependent
on object compliance, and hence information from cutaneous mechanoreceptors
is sufficient for discrimination of subtle differences in compliance.
When the surface is rigid, kinesthetic information is necessary for discrimination,
and the discriminability is much poorer than that for objects with deformable
surfaces. The mechanistic data for rubber specimens indicates that the
basis for the perception of softness of rubber-like objects is likely
to be the spatio-temporal variation of pressure on the skin (or, equivalently
the skin displacement and its derivatives). Neurophysiological data shows
that the resulting responses from slowly adapting type I afferent population
within the skin might encode the compliance of such objects.
|